How to Cite
Peña V. A., & Gómez-Mejía A. (2020). Effect of heuristic anchoring and adjustment, and optimism bias, in stock market forecasts. Revista Finanzas y Política Económica, 11(2), 389-409. Retrieved from https://revfinypolecon.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/2920

Abstract

Stock market forecasting is an important and challenging process that influences investment decisions. This paper presents an experimental design that aims to measure the influence of the anchoring and adjustment heuristic and optimism bias in these forecasts. The study was conducted using information from the S&P MILA Pacific Alliance Select financial index; this was presented to 670 students from the cities of Concepción (Chile), Cali (Colombia), and Lima (Peru). Data was collected and presented through an instrument that asked participants to make a forecast judgment of the said financial index, based on the presented graphics, representing a year, a month, a week, and the last closing value of the index. Thus, it was possible to measure the influence of the anchor and adjustment heuristic in order to establish whether the presence of an initial value affected the financial forecast. Similarly, the study sought to determine whether the judgment issued was biased toward an optimistic or pessimistic position, thereby proving the presence of an error or expectation bias, known as optimism bias. The results were analyzed using the least squares method, and the data panel confirmed that the anchoring and adjustment heuristic influences the forecast of the financial index used in the study. Similarly, the presence of optimism bias in the cognitive process of forecasting in finance was inferred.

Licence

Copyright (c) 2020 Revista Finanzas y Política Económica

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

References

1. Aguiar, F. (2004). Teoría de la decisión e incertidumbre: modelos normativos y descriptivos. Empiria:
Revista de Metodología de Ciencias Sociales, (8), 139-160. https://doi.org//10.5944/empiria.8.2004.982

2. Amir, E., & Ganzach, Y. (1998). Overreaction and underreaction in analysts’ forecasts. Journal of Economic
Behavior & Organization, 37(3), 333-347. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.1024.5590&rep=rep1&type=pdf

3. Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2003). «Coherent Arbitrariness»: Stable Demand Curves
Without Stable Preferences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(1), 73-106. https://doi.org/10.1162/00335530360535153

4. Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2002). When predictions fail: The dilemma of unrealistic optimism. In
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. W., & Kahneman, D. (eds.). Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment.
(pp. 334-347). New York: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org//10.1017/CBO9780511808098.021

5. Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (2001). Boys will be boys: Gender, overconfidence, and common stock investment.
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 261-292. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.139415

6. Barberis, N., & Thaler, R. (2003). A survey of behavioral finance. Handbook of the Economic of Finance,
1052-1121. http://houdekpetr.cz/!data/public_html/papers/economics_psychology/Barberis%20Thaler%202003.pdf

7. Ben-David, I., & Hirshleifer, D. (2012). Are investors really reluctant to realize their losses? Trading responses
to past returns and the disposition effect. Review of Financial Studies. 25(8), 2485-2532. https://doi.org/10.1093/rfs/hhs077

8. Blankenship, K. L., Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., Detweiler-Bedell, B., & Macy, C. L. (2008). Elaboration
and consequences of anchored estimates: An attitudinal perspective on numerical anchoring. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 44(6), 1465-1476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.005

9. Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the «planning fallacy»: Why people underestimate
their task completion times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 366-381. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.366

10. Cadet, B., & Chasseigne, G. (2009). Psychologie du jugement et de la décision: Des modèles aux applications.
(A. Editore, Ed.) (1st ed). Paris: De Boeck Universite.

11. Campbell, S. D., & Sharpe, S. A. (2009). Anchoring bias in consensus forecasts and its effect on
market prices. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, 44(2), 369-390. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022109009090127

12. Caputo, A. (2014). Relevant information, personality traits and anchoring effect. International Journal of
Management and Decision Making, 13(1), 62-76. https://doi.org/10.1504/ijmdm.2014.058470

13. Cen, L., Hilary, G., & Wei, K. C. J. (2013). The role of anchoring bias in the equity market: Evidence
from analysts’ earnings forecasts and stock returns. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, 48(1),
47-76. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002210901200060

14. Chambers, J. R., & Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in Social Comparative Judgments: The Role of
Nonmotivated Factors in Above-Average and Comparative-Optimism Effects. Psychological Bulletin,
130(5), 813-838. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813

15. Chang, E. C., Lin, T. C., Luo, Y., & Ren, J. (2019). Ex-day returns of stock distributions: An anchoring
explanation. Management Science. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2017.2843

16. Chapman, G. B., & Johnson, E. J. (1999). Anchoring, activation, and the construction of values.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79(2), 115-153. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1999.2841

17. Chapman, G. B., & Johnson, E. J. (2002). Incorporating the Irrelevant: Anchors in Judgments of Belief
and Value. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. W., & Kahneman, D. (eds.). Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of
Intuitive Judgment (pp. 120-138). New York: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/dx.doi.org/10.1017/
CBO9780511808098.008

18. Cowles, A., & Jones, H. E. (1937). Some A Posteriori Probabilities in Stock Market Action. Econometrica,
5(3), 280-294. https://doi.org/10.2307/1905515

19. Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., & Teoh, S. H. (2002). Investor psychology in capital markets: evidence
and policy implication. Journal of Monetary Economics, 49(1), 139-209. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3932(01)0001-5

20. De Bondt, W. F. M., & Thaler, R. (1985). Does the Stock Market Overreact? Journal of Finance, 40(3),
793-805. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1985.tb05004.x

21. Duclos, R. (2014). The psychology of investment behavior: (De)biasing financial decision-making one
graph at a time. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 317-325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2014.11.005

22. Ductor, L., & Leiva-Leon, D. (2016). Dynamics of global business cycle interdependence. Journal of
International Economics, 102, 110-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jinteco.2016.07.003

23. Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1981). Behavioral Decision Theory: Processed of Judgment and Choice.
Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 53-88. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.32.020181.000413

24. Englich, B., & Soder, K. (2009). Moody experts—How mood and expertise influence judgmental anchoring.
Judgment and Decision Making, 4(1), 41-50. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2009-02933-004

25. Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Putting adjustment back in the anchoring and adjustment heuristic:
Differential Processing of Self-Generated and Experimenter-Provided Anchors. Psychological Science,
12(5), 391-396. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00372

26. Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The Anchoring-and- Adjustment Heuristic Why the Adjustments.
Psychological Science, 17(4), 311-318. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01704.x

27. Fama, E. (1997). Market Efficiency, Long-Term Returns, and Behavioral Finance. Journal of Financial
Economics, 49(3), 283-306. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/0187/0cadeddf6c98ece3b878ce168c86636a5c4b.pdf

28. Fama, E. F. (1970). Efficient Capital Markets: A Review of Theory and Empirical Work. The Journal of
Finance, 25(2), 383. https://doi.org/10.2307/2325486

29. Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford University Press.

30. Fisher, K. L., & Statman, M. (2000). Cognitive Biases in Market Forecasts. The Journal of Portfolio
Management, 27(1), 72-81. https://doi.org/10.3905/jpm.2000.319785

31. Forbes, W., Hudson, R., Skerratt, L., & Soufian, M. (2015). Which heuristics can aid financial-decisionmaking?
International Review of Financial Analysis, 42, 199-210.

32. Galinsky, A. D., & Mussweiler, T. (2001). First offers as anchors: The role of perspective-taking
and negotiator focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 657-669. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.657

33. George, T. J., & Hwang, C. Y. (2004). The 52-week high and momentum investing. Journal of Finance,
59(5), 2145-2176. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.2004.00695.x

34. Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1),
451–482. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346

35. Giordani, P., & Söderlind, P. (2006). Is there evidence of pessimism and doubt in subjective distributions?
Implications for the equity premium puzzle. Journal of Economic Dynamics and Control, 30(6), 1027-1043.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jedc.2005.05.001

36. Griffin, D., & Tversky, A. (1992). The weighing of evidence and the determinats of confidence. Cognitive
Psychology, 24, 411-435. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(92)90013-R

37. Hastie, R., & Dawes, R. M. (2010). Rational Choice in an Uncertain World: The Psychology of Judgement
and Decision Making. SAGE Publications.

38. Helweg-Larsen, M., & Shepperd, J. A. (2001). Do Moderators of the Optimistic Bias Affect Personal or
Target Risk Estimates? A Review of the Literature. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(1), 74-95.
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_5

39. Hilary, G., & Hsu, C. (2011). Endogenous overconfidence in managerial forecasts. Journal of Accounting
and Economics, 51(3), 300-313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jacceco.2011.01.002

40. Hirshleifer, D. (2015). Behavioral Finance. Annual Review of Financial Economics, 7(1), 133-159. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-financial-092214-043752

41. Hysenbelli, D., Rubaltelli, E., & Rumiati, R. (2013). Others’ opinions count, but not all of them: anchoring
to ingroup versus outgroup members’ behavior in charitable giving. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(6),
678-690. http://journal.sjdm.org/12/121219/jdm121219.pdf

42. Jefferson, A., Bortolotti, L., & Kuzmanovic, B. (2017). What is unrealistic optimism? Consciousness and
Cognition, 50, 3-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.10.005

43. Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of Bounded Rationality: Psychology for Behavioral Economics. American
Economic Review, 93(5), 1449-1475. https://doi.org/10.1257/000282803322655392

44. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

45. Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1991). Anomalies: The endowment effect, loss aversion,
and status quo bias. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5(1), 193-206. http://pubs.aeaweb.org/
doi/10.1257/jep.5.1.193.

46. Kahneman, D., & Riepe, M. W. (1998). Aspects of Investor Psychology. The Journal of Portfolio Management,
24(4), 52-65. https://doi.org/10.3905/jpm.1998.409643

47. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgement Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases.
Cambridge University Press.

48. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80(4), 237-
251. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034747

49. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica,
47(2), 263. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185

50. Kaustia, M., Alho, E., & Puttonen, V. (2008). How Much Does Expertise Reduce Behavioral Biases? The
Case of Anchoring Effects in Stock Return Estimates. Financial Management, 37(3), 391-412. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-053X.2008.00018.x

51. Kendall, M. G., & Hill, A. B. (1953). The analysis of economic time-series-Part I: Prices. Journal of the
Royal Statistical Society Series A, 116(1), 11. https://doi.org/10.2307/2980947

52. Kinari, Y. (2016). Properties of expectation biases: Optimism and overconfidence. Journal of Behavioral
and Experimental Finance, 10(1), 32-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbef.2016.02.003

53. Krch, D. (2011). Cognitive Processing. In J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, & B. Caplan (eds.). Encyclopedia of
Clinical Neuropsychology. (p. 627). New York, NY: Springer New York.

54. Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311-328.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.311

55. Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All Frames Are Not Created Equal: A Typology and
Critical Analysis of Framing Effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149-188.
https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1998.2804

56. Liao, L.-C., Chou, R. Y., & Chiu, B. (2013). Anchoring effect on foreign institutional investors’ momentum
trading behavior: Evidence from the Taiwan stock market. The North American Journal of Economics and
Finance, 26, 72-91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.najef.2013.07.001

57. Locke, J. (1796). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. The Philosophical Review. 3(5), 601-605.
https://openpublishing.psu.edu/locke/bib/ch0e.html

58. Lovallo, D., & Kahneman, D. (2003). Delusions of Success: How Optimism Undermines Executives’
Decisions. Harvard Business Review. 81(7), 56-63, 117. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12858711

59. Lucas, R. E. J. (1978). Asset Prices in an Exchange Economy. Econometrica, 46(6), 1429-1445. http://www.its.caltech.edu/~pbs/expfinance/Readings/Lucas1978.pdf

60. Lucey, M. E., & O’Connor, F. A. (2016). Mind the gap: Psychological barriers in gold and silver prices.
Finance Research Letters, 17, 135-140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.frl.2016.03.009

61. March, J. G. (1978). Bounded Rationality, Ambiguity, and the Engineering of Choice. The Bell Journal of
Economics, 9(2), 587. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3003600

62. Marsden, A., Veeraraghavan, M., & Ye, M. (2008). Heuristics of Representativeness, Anchoring and
Adjustment, and Leniency: Impact on Earnings’ Forecasts by Australian Analysts. Quarterly Journal of
Finance & Accounting, 47(2), 83-102. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.996514

63. McElroy, T., & Dowd, K. (2007). Susceptibility to anchoring effects: How openness-to-experience influences
responses to anchoring cues. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(1), 48-53. http://journal.sjdm.org/jdm06143.pdf

64. Mintzberg, H., Raisinghani, D., & Theoret, A. (1976). The Structure of «Unstructured» Decision Processes.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 21(2), 246. https://doi.org/10.2307/2392045

65. Montier, J. (2013). Behavioural Investing: A Practitioner’s Guide to Applying Behavioural Finance. Chichester,
England: John Wiley & Sons.

66. Mullainathan, S., & Thaler, R. (2000). Behavioral Economics. the National Bureau of Economic Research
(Vol. 3). Cambridge, MA. https://doi.org/10.3386/w7948

67. Mussweiler, T., Strack, F., & Pfeiffer, T. (2000). Overcoming the Inevitable Anchoring Effect: Considering
the Opposite Compensates for Selective Accessibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9),
1142-1150. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672002611010

68. Newell, B. R., & Bröder, A. (2008). Cognitive processes, models and metaphors in decision research.
Judgment and Decision Making, 3(3), 195-204. http://journal.sjdm.org/bn1.pdf

69. Northcraft, G. B., & Neale, M. A. (1987). Experts, amateurs, and real estate: An anchoring-and-adjustment
perspective on property pricing decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39(1),
84-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(87)90046-X

70. Odean, T. (1998). Are Investors Reluctant to Realize Their Losses? The Journal of Finance, 53(5), 1775-
1798. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-1082.00072

71. Pachur, T., & Bröder, A. (2013). Judgment: a cognitive processing perspective. Wiley Interdisciplinary
Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(6), 665-681. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1259

72. Plous, S. (1989). Thinking the Unthinkable: The Effects of Anchoring on Likelihood Estimates of Nuclear
War. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19(1), 67-91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1989.tb01221.x

73. Pompian, M. M. (2012). Behavioral Finance and Wealth Management : How to Build Optimal Portfolios That
Account for Investor Biases (2.a ed.). Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons.

74. Puri, M., & Robinson, D. (2007). Optimism and economic choice. Journal of Financial Economics, 86(1),
71-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfineco.2006.09.003

75. Rau, R. (2011). Market Inefficiency. In Baker, H. K., & Nofsinger, J. R. (eds.). Behavioral Finance: Investors,
Corporations, and Markets (pp. 331-349). Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons.

76. Ricciardi, V. (2008). The Psychology of Risk: The Behavioral Finance Perspective. In Fabozzi, F. J. (ed.).
The Handbook of Finance. Vol. 2. Investment Management and Financial Management. (pp. 85-111). London:
John Wiley & Sons.

77. Ricciardi, V., & Simon, H. K. (2000). What is Behavioral Finance? Business, Education & Technology
Journal, 2(2), 1-9. https://ssrn.com/abstract=256754

78. Ritter, J. R. (2003). Behavioral finance. Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, 11(4), 429-437. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0927-538X(03)00048-9

79. S&P Global. (2019). S&P MILA Indices Methodology. https://us.spindices.com/documents/methodologies/
methodology-sp-mila-indices.pdf?force_download=true

80. Samuelson, W., & Zeckhauser, R. (1988). Status quo bias in decision making. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty,
1(1), 7-59. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00055564

81. Schwert, G. W. (2003). Anomalies and Market Efficiency. Handbook of the Economics of Finance, 1, 939-
974. http://schwert.ssb.rochester.edu/hbfech15.pdf

82. Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Current Biology, 21(23), R941-R945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
cub.2011.10.030

83. Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism
bias. Nature, 450(November), 102-105. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17960136

84. Shefrin, H., & Statman, M. (2000). Behavioral Portfolio Theory. The Journal of Financial and Quantitative
Analysis, 35(2), 127. https://doi.org/10.2307/2676187

85. Shepperd, J. A., Carroll, P., Grace, J., & Terry, M. (2002). Exploring the Causes of Comparative Optimism.
Psychologica Belgica, 42, 65-98. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2002-06881-007

86. Shiller, R. J. (2003). From Efficient Markets Theory to Behavioral Finance The 1980s and Excess Volatility.
Journal of Economic Perspectives, 17(1), 83-104. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533003321164967

87. Shin, H., & Park, S. (2018). Do foreign investors mitigate anchoring bias in stock market? Evidence
based on post-earnings announcement drift. Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, 48(December 2016), 224-240.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pacfin.2018.02.008

88. Simon, H. A. (1955). A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1),
99. https://doi.org/10.2307/1884852

89. Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man; social and rational. Operations Research (Vol. 5). Oxford, England: Wiley.

90. Simon, H. A. (1972). Theories of Bounded Rationality. In Decision and Organization: A volume in honor
of Jacob Marschak (pp. 161-176). North-Holland Publishing Company.

91. Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1971). Comparison of Bayesian and regression approaches to the study
of information processing in judgment. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 6(6), 649-744.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(71)90033-X

92. Slovic, Paul. (1972). Psychological Study of Human Judgment: Implications for Investment Decision Making.
Journal of Finance, 27(4), 779-799. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1972.
tb01311.x

93. Staněk, R. (2017). Home bias in sport betting : Evidence from Czech betting market. Judgment and Decision
Making, 12(2), 168-172. http://journal.sjdm.org/16/16824/jdm16824.html7

94. Thaler, R. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior and
Organization, 1(1), 39-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(80)90051-7

95. Thaler, R., & Sunstein, C. (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness (revised).
Penguin UK.

96. Thompson, S. C., Armstrong, W., & Thomas, C. (1998). Illusions of Control, Underestimations,
and Accuracy: A Control Heuristic Explanation. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 143-161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.143

97. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science,
185(4157), 1124-1131. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124

98. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science,
211(4481), 453-458. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683

99. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in Prospect Theory: Cumulative Representation of
Uncertainty. Journal of Risk & Uncertainty, 5(4), 297-323. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF00122574

100. Von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of Games and Economic Behavior. Princeton University
Press.

101. Wansink, B., Kent, R. J., & Hoch, S. J. (1998). An Anchoring and Adjustment Model of Purchase Quantity
Decisions. Journal of Marketing Research, 35(1), 71. https://doi.org/10.2307/3151931

102. Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 39(5), 806-820. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806

103. Westerhoff, F. (2003). Anchoring and Psychological Barriers in Foreign Exchange Markets. Journal of
Behavioral Finance, 4(2), 65-70. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15427579JPFM0402_03

104. Wilke, A., & Mata, R. (2012). Cognitive Bias. In Encyclopedia of human behavior (pp. 531-535). London:
Academic Press.

105. Working, H. (1934). A Random-Difference Series for Use in the Analysis of Time Series. Journal of the
American Statistical Association, 29(185), 11-24. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/01621459.1934.10502683
Sistema OJS 3 - Metabiblioteca |